全文获取类型
收费全文 | 11410篇 |
免费 | 196篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
管理学 | 1635篇 |
民族学 | 44篇 |
人才学 | 1篇 |
人口学 | 933篇 |
丛书文集 | 52篇 |
理论方法论 | 1138篇 |
综合类 | 173篇 |
社会学 | 5142篇 |
统计学 | 2489篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 65篇 |
2020年 | 160篇 |
2019年 | 257篇 |
2018年 | 245篇 |
2017年 | 369篇 |
2016年 | 291篇 |
2015年 | 234篇 |
2014年 | 259篇 |
2013年 | 1965篇 |
2012年 | 373篇 |
2011年 | 309篇 |
2010年 | 284篇 |
2009年 | 256篇 |
2008年 | 337篇 |
2007年 | 340篇 |
2006年 | 278篇 |
2005年 | 265篇 |
2004年 | 240篇 |
2003年 | 224篇 |
2002年 | 245篇 |
2001年 | 268篇 |
2000年 | 239篇 |
1999年 | 206篇 |
1998年 | 179篇 |
1997年 | 193篇 |
1996年 | 177篇 |
1995年 | 151篇 |
1994年 | 131篇 |
1993年 | 173篇 |
1992年 | 166篇 |
1991年 | 150篇 |
1990年 | 169篇 |
1989年 | 144篇 |
1988年 | 139篇 |
1987年 | 138篇 |
1986年 | 121篇 |
1985年 | 117篇 |
1984年 | 150篇 |
1983年 | 117篇 |
1982年 | 124篇 |
1981年 | 91篇 |
1980年 | 115篇 |
1979年 | 134篇 |
1978年 | 89篇 |
1977年 | 93篇 |
1976年 | 95篇 |
1975年 | 97篇 |
1974年 | 82篇 |
1973年 | 68篇 |
1972年 | 67篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
When do infants begin to communicate positive affect about physical objects to their social partners? We examined developmental changes in the timing of smiles during episodes of initiating joint attention that involved an infant gazing between an object and a social partner. Twenty‐six typically developing infants were observed at 8, 10, and 12 months during the Early Social‐Communication Scales, a semistructured assessment for eliciting initiating joint attention and related behaviors. The proportion of infant smiling during initiating joint attention episodes did not change with age, but there was a change in the timing of the smiles. The likelihood of infants smiling at an object and then gazing at the experimenter while smiling (anticipatory smiling) increased between 8 and 10 months and remained stable between 10 and 12 months. The increase in the number of infants who smiled at an object and then made eye contact suggests a developing ability to communicate positive affect about an object. 相似文献
62.
63.
64.
This article reports an ethnographic study of pre‐school children's social knowledge domains. Results show how the children's shared knowledge concerning social status and social differentiation in the group was strengthened and confirmed during a traditional teacher‐led ring game, where they were supposed to choose a friend. We suggest that from the perspective of the children, the game situation may be understood as, at the same time, a space of participation and an arena for establishing the power order. From the perspective of the teacher, pedagogical intentions may be challenged as they encounter and are mediated and interpreted by children's peer‐cultures. 相似文献
65.
66.
The benchmark dose (BMD) is an exposure level that would induce a small risk increase (BMR level) above the background. The BMD approach to deriving a reference dose for risk assessment of noncancer effects is advantageous in that the estimate of BMD is not restricted to experimental doses and utilizes most available dose-response information. To quantify statistical uncertainty of a BMD estimate, we often calculate and report its lower confidence limit (i.e., BMDL), and may even consider it as a more conservative alternative to BMD itself. Computation of BMDL may involve normal confidence limits to BMD in conjunction with the delta method. Therefore, factors, such as small sample size and nonlinearity in model parameters, can affect the performance of the delta method BMDL, and alternative methods are useful. In this article, we propose a bootstrap method to estimate BMDL utilizing a scheme that consists of a resampling of residuals after model fitting and a one-step formula for parameter estimation. We illustrate the method with clustered binary data from developmental toxicity experiments. Our analysis shows that with moderately elevated dose-response data, the distribution of BMD estimator tends to be left-skewed and bootstrap BMDL s are smaller than the delta method BMDL s on average, hence quantifying risk more conservatively. Statistically, the bootstrap BMDL quantifies the uncertainty of the true BMD more honestly than the delta method BMDL as its coverage probability is closer to the nominal level than that of delta method BMDL. We find that BMD and BMDL estimates are generally insensitive to model choices provided that the models fit the data comparably well near the region of BMD. Our analysis also suggests that, in the presence of a significant and moderately strong dose-response relationship, the developmental toxicity experiments under the standard protocol support dose-response assessment at 5% BMR for BMD and 95% confidence level for BMDL. 相似文献
67.
Don A. Dillman Tommy L. Brown John E. Carlson Edwin H. Carpenter Frederick O. Lorenz Robert Mason John Saltiel Roberta L. Songster 《Rural sociology》1995,60(4):674-687
Abstract Past research suggests that mail surveys encourage a primacy effect, which is a tendency to choose the first answers from a list, whereas telephone surveys encourage a recency effect, a tendency to choose the last answers from a list. This paper summarizes results from 82 new experiments conducted in 12 separate surveys in seven states. Only four of 33 mail survey comparisons exhibited significant primacy effects, while five of 26 experiments in telephone surveys exhibited recency effects. In addition, only three of 23 cross-method comparisons produced a significant primacy/recency effect in the expected manner. The conclusion is that the prevalence of primacy and recency effects has been over-estimated by past research and a new theoretical approach that takes into account multiple causation is needed for examining these effects. 相似文献
68.
Surprisingly little research has sought to explain differential participation after recruitment into social movement organizations (SMOs). This study examines the influence of several sets of predictors on participation by members of a national organization in the antihunger movement. The findings highlight the importance of incentive, ideological, and microstructural factors for differential participation and suggest that favorable perceptions of SMOs also promote participation. Final remarks address the implications of the findings for the social movement and voluntary organization literatures. 相似文献
69.
Peter R. Gross 《Work and stress》1994,8(1):68-74
Although empathy has been found to distinguish effective counsellors from less effective counsellors, human services professionals who continually experience negative emotional reactions to the feelings that clients express in counselling may be at risk of developing burnout. This study examined the hypothesis that emotional empathy rather than cognitive empathy is associated with burnout. The sample of human services professionals consisted of 71 Salvation Army Officers. Multiple regression was used to investigate whether the dimensions of empathy could predict three aspects of burnout: personal accomplishment, emotional exhaustion and depersonalization. The first regression analysis indicated that personal distress and empathic concern were significant predictor of personal accomplishment. The second analysis indicated that personal distress was a significant predictor of emotional exhaustion. The third indicated that empathic concern was a significant predictor of depersonalization. Although these results support the hypothesis, personal distress was negatively associated with personal accomplishment, whereas empathic concern was positively associated with personal accomplishment. The small size of the sample may have influenced the reliability of the findings. A multidimensional approach to the role of empathy in counselling may help human services professionals to manage their emotional reactions when they engage in counselling with distressed clients. 相似文献
70.
Regina Thompson Paul H. Templet John K. Gamman Scott T. McCleary Margaret A. Reams 《Risk analysis》1994,14(5):857-861
The goal of Louisiana's 1990–1991 comparative risk project, also called the Louisiana Environmental Action Plan (LEAP), was to incorporate risk assessment into state environmental planning and policymaking. Scientists, government officials, and citizens were brought together to estimate the relative risk to human health, natural resources, and quality of life posed by 33 selected environmental issues. The issues were then ranked according to their relative estimated risks. It was hoped that this ranking of "comparative risks" would enable state policymakers to target the most important environmental problems and allocate scarce public resources more rationally and efficiently. As a result of the project, the governor issued an Executive Order forming a permanent Public Advisory Committee to continue this type of comparative risk assessment in Louisiana. 相似文献