首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   676篇
  免费   30篇
管理学   33篇
民族学   8篇
人才学   1篇
人口学   47篇
丛书文集   1篇
理论方法论   81篇
综合类   2篇
社会学   488篇
统计学   45篇
  2023年   4篇
  2021年   3篇
  2020年   13篇
  2019年   15篇
  2018年   17篇
  2017年   28篇
  2016年   21篇
  2015年   16篇
  2014年   21篇
  2013年   124篇
  2012年   31篇
  2011年   40篇
  2010年   21篇
  2009年   19篇
  2008年   30篇
  2007年   21篇
  2006年   23篇
  2005年   27篇
  2004年   25篇
  2003年   26篇
  2002年   18篇
  2001年   14篇
  2000年   11篇
  1999年   14篇
  1998年   3篇
  1997年   9篇
  1996年   10篇
  1995年   9篇
  1994年   11篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   7篇
  1991年   6篇
  1990年   6篇
  1989年   7篇
  1988年   4篇
  1987年   5篇
  1986年   3篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   10篇
  1983年   4篇
  1982年   4篇
  1981年   4篇
  1980年   1篇
  1979年   2篇
  1978年   4篇
  1977年   1篇
  1976年   2篇
  1973年   1篇
  1970年   1篇
  1969年   3篇
排序方式: 共有706条查询结果,搜索用时 656 毫秒
701.
Policies and programs designed to serve children and families are sometimes misaligned with developmental science research. Broad child neglect reporting laws, first adopted by the United States in 1974, have led to families being prosecuted by child protection authorities for allowing children to participate in everyday age-appropriate activities unsupervised. In this report we describe the challenges of defining child neglect and outline the current landscape of neglect laws in the United States. We then provide a broad overview of some of the developmental milestones children need to reach to participate in unsupervised activities and the benefits of independent activities on child development. Children can often accomplish tasks at a much younger age than law, parents, and caregivers in the U.S. believe. We then turn to the literature from across the world and argue that culture, not innate ability, drives much of the variation in the age at which children can do things on their own. Finally, we make recommendations to parents, caregivers, legislators, advocates, and developmental scientists to better align practice with research. This is a social justice issue that should resonate across party, racial, and class lines. Developmental scientists are needed as advocates and advisors on policies impacting children and families, especially child neglect laws.  相似文献   
702.
In this article, we investigate the (R, S) periodic review, order‐up‐to level inventory control system with stochastic demand and variable leadtimes. Variable leadtimes can lead to order crossover, in which some orders arrive out of sequence. Most theoretical studies of order‐up‐to inventory systems under variable leadtimes assume that crossovers do not occur and, in so doing, overestimate the standard deviation of the realized leadtime distribution and prescribe policies that can inflate inventory costs. We develop a new analytic model of the expected costs associated with this system, making use of a novel approximation of the realized (reduced) leadtime standard deviation resulting from order crossovers. Extensive experimentation through simulation shows that our model closely approximates the true expected cost and can be used to find values of R and S that provide an expected cost close to the minimum cost. Taking account of, as opposed to ignoring, crossovers leads, on average, to substantial improvements in accuracy and significant cost reductions. Our results are particularly useful for managers seeking to reduce inventory costs in supply chains with variable leadtimes.  相似文献   
703.
Variation in infants’ home environment is implicated in their cognitive and psycho-social development. The pandemic has intensified variations in home environments through exacerbating socioeconomic inequalities, and increasing psychological stressors for some families. This study investigates the effects of parental (predominantly maternal) mental health, enriching activities and screen use on 280 24- to 52-month-olds’ executive functions, internalising and externalising problems, and pro-social behaviour; with socioeconomic status and social support as contextual factors. Our results indicate that aspects of the home environment are differentially associated with children’s cognitive and psycho-social development. Parents who experienced sustained mental distress during the pandemic tended to report higher child externalising and internalising problems, and executive function difficulties at follow-up. Children who spent more time engaged in enriching activities with their parents showed stronger executive functions and social competence six months later. Screen use levels during the first year of the pandemic were not associated with outcomes. To mitigate the risk of persistent negative effects for this ‘pandemic generation’ of infants, our study highlights the importance of supporting parents’ mental health. As our results demonstrate the impact of social support on mental health, investing in support services and interventions promoting building support networks are likely to be beneficial.  相似文献   
704.
705.
The relationships that youth experiencing homelessness (YEH) have with supportive family members (i.e. those who provide them with social support) may be protective against risk behaviours and buffer adverse health outcomes. However, little is known about the nature of YEH's supportive family relationships and the type of social support that these family relationships offer them. Thus, we conducted in-depth interviews with 30 YEH to examine the type of social support received from family relationships and to explore the social dynamics of these relationships. We found that many YEH not only continued to maintain relationships with family members after becoming homeless but also received emotional and/or instrumental support from these family members. Beyond providing this type of social support, family members served as a source of motivation for these youth. Additionally, YEH contributed to their family relationships and gained a sense of connectedness in return. Lastly, we found that YEH's family support was influenced by their family circumstances and their need for autonomy. Collectively, our findings suggest there may be a need for interventions designed to strengthen relationships with supportive family members among YEH, who may derive multiple positive benefits from relating with the ‘hidden champions’ in their lives.  相似文献   
706.
How much do migrants stand to gain in income from moving across borders? Answering this question is complicated by non‐random selection of migrants from the general population, which makes it hard to obtain an appropriate comparison group of non‐migrants. New Zealand allows a quota of Tongans to immigrate each year with a random ballot used to choose among the excess number of applicants. A unique survey conducted by the authors allows experimental estimates of the income gains from migration to be obtained by comparing the incomes of migrants to those who applied to migrate, but whose names were not drawn in the ballot, after allowing for the effect of non‐compliance among some of those whose names were drawn. We also conducted a survey of individuals who did not apply for the ballot. Comparing this non‐applicant group to the migrants enables assessment of the degree to which non‐experimental methods can provide an unbiased estimate of the income gains from migration. We find evidence of migrants being positively selected in terms of both observed and unobserved skills. As a result, non‐experimental methods other than instrumental variables are found to overstate the gains from migration by 20–82%, with difference‐in‐differences and bias‐adjusted matching estimators performing best among the alternatives to instrumental variables. (JEL: J61, F22, C21)  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号