全文获取类型
收费全文 | 15347篇 |
免费 | 302篇 |
国内免费 | 15篇 |
专业分类
管理学 | 1908篇 |
民族学 | 103篇 |
人才学 | 1篇 |
人口学 | 1381篇 |
丛书文集 | 151篇 |
理论方法论 | 1391篇 |
综合类 | 723篇 |
社会学 | 7937篇 |
统计学 | 2069篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 84篇 |
2022年 | 73篇 |
2021年 | 119篇 |
2020年 | 252篇 |
2019年 | 316篇 |
2018年 | 413篇 |
2017年 | 582篇 |
2016年 | 417篇 |
2015年 | 321篇 |
2014年 | 385篇 |
2013年 | 2435篇 |
2012年 | 532篇 |
2011年 | 529篇 |
2010年 | 449篇 |
2009年 | 373篇 |
2008年 | 453篇 |
2007年 | 463篇 |
2006年 | 446篇 |
2005年 | 405篇 |
2004年 | 330篇 |
2003年 | 289篇 |
2002年 | 310篇 |
2001年 | 389篇 |
2000年 | 313篇 |
1999年 | 308篇 |
1998年 | 238篇 |
1997年 | 191篇 |
1996年 | 180篇 |
1995年 | 197篇 |
1994年 | 190篇 |
1993年 | 188篇 |
1992年 | 210篇 |
1991年 | 214篇 |
1990年 | 200篇 |
1989年 | 176篇 |
1988年 | 194篇 |
1987年 | 216篇 |
1986年 | 164篇 |
1985年 | 178篇 |
1984年 | 194篇 |
1983年 | 166篇 |
1982年 | 168篇 |
1981年 | 139篇 |
1980年 | 123篇 |
1979年 | 134篇 |
1978年 | 117篇 |
1977年 | 93篇 |
1976年 | 104篇 |
1975年 | 124篇 |
1974年 | 92篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
41.
Cathryn L. Booth K. Alison Clarke‐Stewart Deborah Lowe Vandell Kathleen McCartney Margaret Tresch Owen 《Journal of marriage and the family》2002,64(1):16-26
Mothers' time‐use patterns were compared in families in which infants spent more than 30 hours per week in child care (In‐Care group; n= 143) versus 0 hours per week (At‐Home group; n= 183) from birth to 6 months of age. In‐Care group mothers spent about 12 fewer hours per week interacting with their infants, for about 32% less time; fathers of these infants were more involved in caregiving. The groups did not differ in the quality of mother‐infant interaction. In the In‐Care group, quantity of interaction was related to greater separation anxiety and concerns about effects of maternal employment. Time‐use data were not related to child outcomes at 15 months of age. Results suggest that the effect of extensive time spent apart on the quantity and quality of mother‐infant interaction may be smaller than anticipated. 相似文献
42.
43.
Studying and Measuring Civility: A Framework, Trends and Scale 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Abbott L. Ferriss 《Sociological inquiry》2002,72(3):376-392
Four essential questions for the study of civility involve developing a definition of the term, determining its effects, establishing trends, and predicting the consequences of civility. A framework for studying it includes the actors, their gender, situations and settings, occupational role requirements, the cultural imperatives defining civility, and the processes through which it is learned. Objective measures of civility in the United States show its variability and change. Four items in the 1996 General Social Survey (GSS) are combined to form a scale of civility. Correlates of the scale show that civility does not differ by gender, color, or region. It is weakly associated with income but is significantly associated with education, occupation, and health. Age, education, and health provide a predictive model of civility. Anger, an emotional aspect of interpersonal exchange, reveals reactions characteristic of civil behavior: waiting for anger to pass before responding, trying to forget the incident, not thinking of revenge, not walking away from the situation, and not yelling or hitting. Hypotheses are proposed for further study that involves age, marital status, occupation, health, and emotional control. 相似文献
44.
Unemployment rates differ widely and persistently across counties. This article examines equilibrium forces related to this geographic disparity with a focus on California. We show that although seasonal variation in employment can account for some of the differences, it cannot explain the total variation. Factors such as educational attainment, age, and gender appear to be strongly related to unemployment differences. Individuals living in higher‐unemployment areas also have a lower propensity to migrate. Because migration is usually considered to be the main equilibrating force, this evidence helps further explain why unemployment rates across counties are persistently different. 相似文献
45.
Amanda L. Baden 《Journal of Social Distress and the Homeless》2002,11(2):167-191
In response to the need for increased understanding of the identity process of transracial adoptees, the Cultural–Racial Identity Model (A. L. Baden & R. J. Steward, 1995) was developed; however, the model has yet to be empirically validated. The model allows distinctions to be made between racial identity and cultural identity, resulting in 16 proposed identities. Identities are based on the degrees to which individuals (1) have knowledge of, awareness of, competence within, and comfort with their own racial group's culture, their parents' racial group's culture, and multiple cultures, and (2) are comfortable with their racial group membership and with those belonging to their own racial group, their parents' racial group, and multiple racial groups. Four dimensions of the model were determined for study: the Adoptee Culture Dimension, the Parental Culture Dimension, the Adoptee Race Dimension, and the Parental Race Dimension. In this study, the Cultural–Racial Identity of transracial adoptees was assessed by a modified version of the Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure (MEIM; J. S. Phinney, 1992). Psychological adjustment was assessed by the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI; L. R. Derogatis & P. A. Cleary, 1977). The sample consisted of 51 transracial adoptees who completed mail survey questionnaires. The exploratory findings supported the Cultural–Racial Identity Model by demonstrating that the modified version of the MEIM successfully yielded variation in the potential Cultural–Racial Identities that the transracial adoptees reported. Findings also did not yield support for differences in psychological adjustment among transracial adoptees having different Cultural–Racial Identities. The implications that the results have for counseling practice and social policy were discussed. 相似文献
46.
Deanna L. Sharpe Joan M. Hermsen Jodi Billings 《Journal of Family and Economic Issues》2002,23(1):51-72
To examine flexible work scheduling of married workers a conceptual framework was developed and tested using the 1997 Current Population Survey Work Schedules Supplement. Odds of flextime use were higher for married males, non-Hispanic whites, those with relatively higher levels of education and income, those with a preschool aged child, residents of the Midwest or West (as compared with the South), managers or professionals, and employees of the federal government. 相似文献
47.
Janet F. Werker Christopher T. Fennell Kathleen M. Corcoran Christine L. Stager 《Infancy》2002,3(1):1-30
What do novice word learners know about the sound of words? Word‐learning tasks suggest that young infants (14 months old) confuse similar‐sounding words, whereas mispronunciation detection tasks suggest that slightly older infants (18–24 months old) correctly distinguish similar words. Here we explore whether the difficulty at 14 months stems from infants' novice status as word learners or whether it is inherent in the task demands of learning new words. Results from 3 experiments support a developmental explanation. In Experiment 1, infants of 20 months learned to pair 2 phonetically similar words to 2 different objects under precisely the same conditions that infants of 14 months (Experiment 2) failed. In Experiment 3, infants of 17 months showed intermediate, but still successful, performance in the task. Vocabulary size predicted word‐learning performance, but only in the younger, less experienced word learners. The implications of these results for theories of word learning and lexical representation are discussed. 相似文献
48.
L. N. Choumak 《Intercultural Education》2002,13(4):451-461
In this article, we look at several issues relating to bilingual education in Latvia, as prescribed by the Latvian Ministry of Education and Science. We examine these issues from the perspective of the past and present situation in Belarus, Russia and the former Soviet Union. It is clear, in our view, that bilingual offers both advantages, but also various drawbacks if it is not implemented in an appropriate way. This applies to both the societal and the personal (cognitive level). Movement towards the dominant languages (in this case Latvian) needs to be viewed as a gradual process. Although each generation of newcomers might start with a passive knowledge of the dominant (second) language, this changes into a fluent grasp of the second language across generations. The original native language will lose its dominance in this process. 相似文献
49.
50.
Adult members of 79 Albanian families who had fled from Kosovo to Macedonia during the immediate postwar period (June–August 1999) were interviewed. In conjunction with therapeutic intervention, the Harvard Trauma Questionnaire (HTQ) and the General Health Questionnaire (GTQ) were administered. Trauma symptoms as measured by the HTQ and psychological distress as measured by the GHQ were highly correlated. The best predictor of psychological distress was severity of the trauma experienced. Refugees with high educational attainment had fewer trauma symptoms than those with lower educational attainment. Refugees who lived with host families in towns showed fewer trauma symptoms than those who lived in camps, but they were also more highly educated. The results of hierarchical multiple regression analyses suggested that educational attainment may mediate the effect of place of residence. Educational attainment may be an indicator of resourceful and effective coping strategies that ameliorate the effects of trauma. 相似文献