首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   76篇
  免费   0篇
管理学   3篇
人口学   1篇
理论方法论   27篇
社会学   44篇
统计学   1篇
  2019年   1篇
  2014年   2篇
  2013年   5篇
  2012年   3篇
  2011年   1篇
  2010年   5篇
  2009年   2篇
  2008年   1篇
  2007年   3篇
  2005年   2篇
  2004年   1篇
  2002年   1篇
  2001年   2篇
  1999年   1篇
  1998年   3篇
  1997年   3篇
  1996年   1篇
  1995年   4篇
  1993年   3篇
  1992年   1篇
  1991年   2篇
  1990年   4篇
  1989年   6篇
  1988年   3篇
  1987年   5篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   1篇
  1983年   1篇
  1982年   1篇
  1981年   1篇
  1978年   1篇
  1977年   2篇
  1972年   2篇
排序方式: 共有76条查询结果,搜索用时 406 毫秒
11.
12.
13.
From a longitudinal survey of a British cohort born in 1958 this study finds that, by age 33, off-spring of parents who divorced are more likely to have dissolved their first partnerships. This finding persists after taking into account age at first partnership, type of first partnership (marital, pre-marital cohabiting union, and cohabiting union), and indicators of class background and childhood and adolescent school achievement and behaviour problems. Some of these factors are associated with partnership dissolution in their own right, but the association between parental divorce and second generation partnership dissolution is largely independent of them. Demographic factors, including type of and age at first partnership, were important links between parental divorce and partnership dissolution. Moreover, the estimated effects of parental divorce were substantially reduced when the demographic variables were taken into account, suggesting that cohabitation and early partnership may be important pathways through which a parental divorce, or the unmeasured characteristics correlated with it, affect partnership dissolution.  相似文献   
14.
Book reviewed in this article: VIC SATZEWICH, Racism and the Incorporation of Foreign Labour: Farm Labour Migration to Canada Since 1945  相似文献   
15.
This article explains how data associated with attitude change and persuasion in mainstream psychology and social psychology can be reinterpreted as evidence of the agentive ability of taking a position. A critical review is made of the elaboration likelihood model and a position model is adapted from positioning theory. The postulates of the elaboration likelihood model are then subjected to critical scrutiny by means of comparison with our position model. In this way regularities associated with attitude change and persuasion are interpreted as evidence of a deliberative process, involving negotiation and the use of supporting and opposing arguments. We present this as evidence of the commonly held agentive ability to position oneself and anticipate the positions of others.  相似文献   
16.
Response-Order Effects and the Early Gallup Split-Ballots   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
A meta-analysis of split-ballots conducted by the Gallup Organizationin the 1930s, 1940s, and early 1950s shows that response-ordereffects were generally small in magnitude when averaged acrossa great variety of topics and questions—and as comparedwith many of those reported in the response-effects literaturetoday. When analyzed by various question characteristics, theresults provided some support for predictions derived from currentcognitive models of response-order effects, particularly thosefrom satisficing theory. As predicted, questions asked orallywere more likely to generate a statistically significant patternof recency effects if the response alternatives or the questionsas a whole were longer rather than shorter. Other predictedpatterns of primacy and recency effects failed to materialize,however, perhaps largely because of the inherent design limitationsand partial confounding of question attributes in any such secondaryanalysis of archival survey data, but perhaps, too, becauseof simple chance variations. The data from these early experimentsnonetheless provide a partial, albeit limited, test of rivalhypotheses and explanations of response-order effects in theliterature.  相似文献   
17.
We examine the effect of giving respondents time to think about their stated choices (SC) in a survey of cholera and typhoid vaccine preferences in Hue, Vietnam. Because neither vaccine is widely available in Vietnam, we used the SC approach (a stated preference technique) and gave half of our respondents overnight to think about their choices to make the hypothetical valuation scenario as similar to a real‐life choice situation as possible. Respondents who were given extra time made fewer choices that violated internal validity tests of utility theory, and had lower average willingness to pay (WTP), confirming a result found in similar studies in the contingent valuation literature. (JEL D12, I18, C25)  相似文献   
18.
We examine how direct to consumer advertising affects the delay between diagnosis and pharmacological treatment for patients suffering from a common chronic disease. The primary data for this study consist of patients diagnosed with osteoarthritis (N = 18,235) taken from a geographically diverse national research network of 72 primary care practices with 348 physicians in 27 states over the 1999–2002 time period. Brand‐specific advertising data were collected for local and network television at the monthly level for the nearest media markets to the practices. Results of duration models of delay to treatment suggest advertising does affect the length of time that patients and physicians wait to initiate therapy. This evidence suggests that these effects may be welfare enhancing in that advertising tends to encourage more rapid adoption among patients who are good clinical candidates for the therapy and leads to less rapid adoption among some patients who are poor clinical candidates. (JEL D12, I11)  相似文献   
19.
Inspiration*     
Au lieu d'écrire, la plupart des sociologues se conferment encore à l'orthodoxie de l'auteur et de l'éloquence en faisant des comptes rendus, en recopiant ou en prenant des notes. En évoquant des expériences d'ecriture bien connues, liées à la mort du sujet connaissant et de l'auteur éloquent, nous voulons montrer que le modèle expressif est seulement persuasif si nous réprimons ce que nos corps écrivants savent de l'inspiration, de la communion, de la méditation, de l'annonciation, du génie, de la prophétie, des anges, des fantômes, de la possession, de l'intuition, de la passion, du ravissement et de l'extase. Ces injonctions transformatives, que les écrivains expérimentent chaque jour, sont les fondements de la créativité et de la socialité. Elles viennent appuyer l'hypothèse de Levinas voulant que la possession ou l'enthousiasme soient dissimulés au coeur de l'écriture et d'autres activity, qu'un délire plus profond que la pensee sous-tende la pensée. Instead of writing, most sociologists still adhere to an authorial and expressive orthodoxy by writing up, writing out or writing down. By evoking familiar writerly experiences of the death of the knowing subject and the expressive author, I want to show that the expressive model is only persuasive if we repress what our writing bodies know of inspiration, communion, muses, annunciation, genius, prophecy, angels, ghosts, possession, intuition, passion, rapture and ecstasy. These transformative summonses, experienced daily by writers, are the bases of creativity and sociality. They support Levinas' suggestion that enthusiasm or possession are concealed at the heart of writing and other activities, that “a delirium more profound than thought” supports thought.  相似文献   
20.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号