首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   319篇
  免费   11篇
管理学   24篇
民族学   3篇
人口学   35篇
理论方法论   51篇
综合类   3篇
社会学   178篇
统计学   36篇
  2024年   1篇
  2023年   1篇
  2021年   3篇
  2020年   3篇
  2019年   5篇
  2018年   13篇
  2017年   14篇
  2016年   14篇
  2015年   5篇
  2014年   11篇
  2013年   59篇
  2012年   19篇
  2011年   15篇
  2010年   11篇
  2009年   4篇
  2008年   11篇
  2007年   10篇
  2006年   11篇
  2005年   9篇
  2004年   13篇
  2003年   6篇
  2002年   8篇
  2001年   8篇
  2000年   5篇
  1999年   11篇
  1998年   6篇
  1997年   6篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   6篇
  1994年   4篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   5篇
  1990年   3篇
  1989年   1篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   2篇
  1986年   4篇
  1985年   2篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   2篇
  1982年   2篇
  1981年   1篇
  1979年   2篇
  1978年   1篇
  1975年   1篇
  1973年   1篇
排序方式: 共有330条查询结果,搜索用时 890 毫秒
81.
Public risk perceptions of mass disasters carry considerable influences, both psychologically and economically, despite their oft‐times imprecise nature. Prior research has identified the presence of an optimistic bias that affects risk perception, but there is a dearth of literature examining how these perceptions differ among cultures—particularly with regard to mass disasters. The present study explores differences among Japanese, Argentinean, and North American mental health workers in their rates of the optimistic bias in risk perceptions as contrasted between natural disasters and terrorist events. The results indicate a significant difference among cultures in levels of perceived risk that do not correspond to actual exposure rates. Japanese groups had the highest risk perceptions for both types of hazards and North Americans and Argentineans had the lowest risk perceptions for terrorism. Additionally, participants across all cultures rated risk to self as lower than risk to others (optimistic bias) across all disaster types. These findings suggest that cultural factors may have a greater influence on risk perception than social exposure, and that the belief that one is more immune to disasters compared to others may be a cross‐cultural phenomenon.  相似文献   
82.
商场女装部,一个正血拼的贵妇喊出我名字,我们有多久没见?五年?还是六年?我赶紧奉上不失礼的笑,打心底认定完全陌生的她十有八九认错人。她接着报出自己姓名和一些关键词,我才恍然记起彼此曾是同事。辞职嫁作商人妇的她妆容精致得像油画,原本黑油油的直发又染又烫。  相似文献   
83.
84.
Risks associated with toxicants in food are often controlled by exposure reduction. When exposure recommendations are developed for foods with both harmful and beneficial qualities, however, they must balance the associated risks and benefits to maximize public health. Although quantitative methods are commonly used to evaluate health risks, such methods have not been generally applied to evaluating the health benefits associated with environmental exposures. A quantitative method for risk-benefit analysis is presented that allows for consideration of diverse health endpoints that differ in their impact (i.e., duration and severity) using dose-response modeling weighted by quality-adjusted life years saved. To demonstrate the usefulness of this method, the risks and benefits of fish consumption are evaluated using a single health risk and health benefit endpoint. Benefits are defined as the decrease in myocardial infarction mortality resulting from fish consumption, and risks are defined as the increase in neurodevelopmental delay (i.e., talking) resulting from prenatal methylmercury exposure. Fish consumption rates are based on information from Washington State. Using the proposed framework, the net health impact of eating fish is estimated in either a whole population or a population consisting of women of childbearing age and their children. It is demonstrated that across a range of fish methylmercury concentrations (0-1 ppm) and intake levels (0-25 g/day), individuals would have to weight the neurodevelopmental effects 6 times more (in the whole population) or 250 times less (among women of child-bearing age and their children) than the myocardial infarction benefits in order to be ambivalent about whether or not to consume fish. These methods can be generalized to evaluate the merits of other public health and risk management programs that involve trade-offs between risks and benefits.  相似文献   
85.
86.
87.
88.
89.
90.
This paper reports the findings from a phenomenological study examining the lived experience of moving to a long-term care facility and the process of socialization for new residents into the nursing home culture and environment. Three residents were followed over the first six months after moving to a long-term care facility using a series of in depth interviews and participant observation with residents themselves, staff members and family members. This paper focuses on the five institutional level processes related to socialization into the long-term care environment as they were experienced by the new residents. These included: placing the body, defining the body, focusing on the body, managing the body, and relating to the body. The findings point to the importance of place in the construction of institutional bodies in the long-term care context.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号