首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   434篇
  免费   15篇
管理学   68篇
民族学   3篇
人口学   38篇
丛书文集   3篇
理论方法论   60篇
综合类   4篇
社会学   224篇
统计学   49篇
  2023年   2篇
  2022年   2篇
  2020年   12篇
  2019年   14篇
  2018年   8篇
  2017年   19篇
  2016年   10篇
  2015年   20篇
  2014年   14篇
  2013年   70篇
  2012年   7篇
  2011年   17篇
  2010年   9篇
  2009年   7篇
  2008年   15篇
  2007年   12篇
  2006年   14篇
  2005年   10篇
  2004年   16篇
  2003年   15篇
  2002年   13篇
  2001年   13篇
  2000年   9篇
  1999年   7篇
  1998年   8篇
  1997年   4篇
  1996年   8篇
  1995年   14篇
  1994年   11篇
  1993年   3篇
  1992年   2篇
  1991年   7篇
  1990年   5篇
  1989年   4篇
  1988年   6篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   4篇
  1984年   6篇
  1983年   4篇
  1981年   2篇
  1980年   2篇
  1978年   4篇
  1977年   4篇
  1975年   2篇
  1972年   1篇
  1971年   1篇
  1969年   2篇
  1968年   1篇
  1964年   1篇
排序方式: 共有449条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
111.
Statistics and Computing - This paper presents tests to formally choose between regression models using different derivatives of a functional covariate in scalar-on-function regression. We...  相似文献   
112.
Theory and Society -  相似文献   
113.
Model dependent and robust test statistics constructed using a generalized estimating equations extension of logistic regression applicable to the analysis of correlated binary outcome data are shown to have relatively simple algebraic expressions in stratified analyses where all variables are measured at the cluster level These expressions are used to demonstrate the close relationship to standard procedures which assume that subjects responses are independent, to prove that the asymptotic validity of model dependent test statistics is assured if the average correlation between cluster members is constant, and that this assumption can be relaxed when there are the same number of subjects in each cluster.  相似文献   
114.
In this second article of a two‐part series, data from three separate studies are presented supporting the reliability, discriminant, and construct validity of the Attraction to Sexual Aggression (ASA) scale. Higher scores on this scale were associated with attitudes supporting aggression against women, affective reactions to media portrayals of forced sex, physiological and self‐reported sexual arousal, hostility toward women, dominance motives, and antisocial personality characteristics. Based on data with low sexual aggressors, higher ASA scores were associated with greater similarity (on relevant dimensions) to high sexual aggressors, suggesting that ASA may help identify potentially sexually aggressive men at “risk” for future sexual coercion. However, caution in the use of such identification is urged. Finally, it is concluded that although the data support the usefulness of earlier work with self‐reported likelihood measures, the use of ASA is an improvement in this line of research. Directions for future development of this scale and research are also discussed.  相似文献   
115.
A simple transformation of classical binomial confidence limits provides exact confidence limits in situations where a confounding variable is present. An example is the multiple-choice test, where a correct answer may represent either knowledge or guesswork, the latter being the confounding variable.  相似文献   
116.
117.
Estimation of Weibull distribution shape and scale parameters is accomplished through use of symmetrically located percentiles from a sample. The process requires algebraic solution of two equations derived from the cumulative distribution function. Three alternatives examined are compared for precision and variability with maximum likelihood (MLE) and least squares (LS) estimators. The best percentile estimator (using the 10th and 90th) is inferior to MLE in variability and to one least squares estimator in accuracy and variability to a small degree. However, application of a correction factor related to sample size improves the percentile estimator substantially, making it more accurate than LS.  相似文献   
118.
Charles Tilly’s work on repertoires of contention and social mechanisms was pathbreaking. In this article, I argue that his understanding of both concepts overlaps with social-theoretical work informed by the philosophical tradition of classical American pragmatism. There is no evidence that Tilly was influenced by pragmatism, but I argue that the overlap is substantial enough that large portions of his oeuvre can serve as illustrations of the explanatory power of pragmatist social science—and that Tilly’s theorization of mechanisms in particular would have been even stronger had he engaged pragmatism directly.  相似文献   
119.
Using Time Intervals Between Expected Events to Communicate Risk Magnitudes   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Because members of the public have difficulty understanding risk presented in terms of odds ratios (e.g., 1 in 1000) and in comparing odds ratios from different hazards, we examined the use of time intervals between expected harmful events to communicate risk. Perceptions of the risk from a hypothetical instance of naturally-occurring, cancer-causing arsenic in drinking water supplies was examined with a sample of 705 homeowners. The risk was described as either 1 in 1000 or 1 in 100,000 and as present in a town of 2000 people or a city of 200,000 people. With these parameters, the time intervals ranged from 1 expected death in 3500 years (1 in 100,000 risk, small town) to 1 death every 4 months (1 in 1000 risk, city). The addition of time intervals to the odds ratios significantly decreased perceived threat and perceived need for action in the small town but did not affect response for the city. These framing effects were nearly as large as a 100-fold difference in actual risk. Instances when this communication approach may be useful are discussed.  相似文献   
120.
Three experimental studies were conducted employing hypothetical news stories to compare the effects on reader risk perceptions of two situations: when agency communication behavior was reported to be responsive to citizens' risk concerns, vs. when the agency was reported to be unresponsive. In the first two experiments, news stories of public meetings filled with distrust and controversy led to ratings indicating greater perceived risk than news stories reporting no distrust or controversy, even though the risk information was held constant. This effect appeared clearly when the differences in meeting tone were extreme and subjects made their ratings from their recall of the stories, but it was much weaker when the differences were moderate and subjects were allowed to go back over the news stories to help separate risk information from conflict information. In the third experiment, news stories about a spill cleanup systematically varied the seriousness of the spill, the amount of technical information provided in the story, and the agency behavior and resulting community outrage. The outrage manipulation significantly affected affective and cognitive components of perceived risk, but not hypothetical behavioral intentions. Seriousness and technical detail had very little effect on perceived risk.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号