首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   527篇
  免费   20篇
管理学   214篇
民族学   1篇
人口学   20篇
丛书文集   1篇
理论方法论   37篇
综合类   1篇
社会学   216篇
统计学   57篇
  2023年   2篇
  2021年   2篇
  2020年   10篇
  2019年   10篇
  2018年   8篇
  2017年   19篇
  2016年   26篇
  2015年   20篇
  2014年   21篇
  2013年   101篇
  2012年   19篇
  2011年   19篇
  2010年   10篇
  2009年   15篇
  2008年   16篇
  2007年   18篇
  2006年   12篇
  2005年   14篇
  2004年   9篇
  2003年   12篇
  2002年   7篇
  2001年   8篇
  2000年   7篇
  1999年   6篇
  1998年   10篇
  1997年   8篇
  1996年   10篇
  1995年   13篇
  1994年   9篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   9篇
  1991年   10篇
  1990年   8篇
  1989年   5篇
  1988年   6篇
  1987年   9篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   6篇
  1984年   4篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   4篇
  1981年   5篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   3篇
  1978年   8篇
  1977年   3篇
  1976年   3篇
  1975年   2篇
  1974年   3篇
  1971年   2篇
排序方式: 共有547条查询结果,搜索用时 8 毫秒
41.
Tom Cox 《Work and stress》2013,27(3):203-206
Abstract

When compared to day workers, night-shift workers report that they eat fewer meals, have poorer appetites, are less satisfied with their eating habits, and eat at different times of the day. When demographic variables are held constant, there do not appear to be any significant differences between experienced day and night-shift workers in caffeine or alcohol consumption. Exceptions to and variations in these findings are probably due to the background influences of social and cultural variables. Eating and drinking behaviours do effect human performance and mood, but little relevant data have been gathered from experienced night-shift workers. There are no shiftworker data evaluating the effectiveness of the special diets or dietary recommendations that have been proposed for use by night-shift workers. Thus, attempts to manipulate night-shift worker eating and drinking habits require experimental test.  相似文献   
42.
Abstract

This paper considers the various issues that frame the development and use of measures of the subjective work environment. It begins by questioning the role of the work environment in determining occupational health, and explores the possible mechanisms by which that environment might exert its influence. It concludes that one of the important final common pathways is psycho-physiological in nature, and is rooted in individual perception and cognition and the experience of stress. Important for this model are the concepts of mediation and moderation. The measurement of the subjective work environment has often been idiosyncratic to the study in hand, and there are few well-established measures in common use. It is argued that researchers should be careful when deciding not to use established measures and effectively ‘invent’ their own. In developing new measures, decisions have to be made concerning the nature of the measure along with its ‘granularity’ and complexity. It is also argued that it is important that researchers should follow good psychometric practice in the development of those measures. Suggestions for ‘good practice’ are discussed. Attention is drawn to the issues of reliability and validity, and this paper discusses the role of triangulation in the planning and execution of data collection and analysis. The paper ends by reviewing the recommendations made towards the development and use of measures of the subjective work environment.  相似文献   
43.
44.
From the Editors     
Tony Cox  Karen Lowrie 《Risk analysis》2013,33(8):1383-1384
  相似文献   
45.
From the Editors     
  相似文献   
46.
Louis Anthony Cox  Jr. 《Risk analysis》2012,32(11):1919-1934
Extreme and catastrophic events pose challenges for normative models of risk management decision making. They invite development of new methods and principles to complement existing normative decision and risk analysis. Because such events are rare, it is difficult to learn about them from experience. They can prompt both too little concern before the fact, and too much after. Emotionally charged and vivid outcomes promote probability neglect and distort risk perceptions. Aversion to acting on uncertain probabilities saps precautionary action; moral hazard distorts incentives to take care; imperfect learning and social adaptation (e.g., herd‐following, group‐think) complicate forecasting and coordination of individual behaviors and undermine prediction, preparation, and insurance of catastrophic events. Such difficulties raise substantial challenges for normative decision theories prescribing how catastrophe risks should be managed. This article summarizes challenges for catastrophic hazards with uncertain or unpredictable frequencies and severities, hard‐to‐envision and incompletely described decision alternatives and consequences, and individual responses that influence each other. Conceptual models and examples clarify where and why new methods are needed to complement traditional normative decision theories for individuals and groups. For example, prospective and retrospective preferences for risk management alternatives may conflict; procedures for combining individual beliefs or preferences can produce collective decisions that no one favors; and individual choices or behaviors in preparing for possible disasters may have no equilibrium. Recent ideas for building “disaster‐resilient” communities can complement traditional normative decision theories, helping to meet the practical need for better ways to manage risks of extreme and catastrophic events.  相似文献   
47.
Dr. Yellman proposes to define frequency as “a time‐rate of events of a specified type over a particular time interval.” We review why no definition of frequency, including this one, can satisfy both of two conditions: (1) the definition should agree with the ordinary meaning of frequency, such as that less frequent events are less likely to occur than more frequent events, over any particular time interval for which the frequencies of both are defined; and (2) the definition should be applicable not only to exponentially distributed times between (or until) events, but also to some nonexponential (e.g., uniformly distributed) times. We make the simple point that no definition can satisfy (1) and (2) by showing that any definition that determines which of any two uniformly distributed times has the higher “frequency” (or that determines that they have the same “frequency,” if neither is higher) must assign a higher frequency number to the distribution with the lower probability of occurrence over some time intervals. Dr. Yellman's proposed phrase, “time‐rate of events … over a particular time interval” is profoundly ambiguous in such cases, as the instantaneous failure rates vary over an infinitely wide range (e.g., from one to infinity), making it unclear which value is denoted by the phrase “time‐rate of events.”  相似文献   
48.
From the Editors     
Tony Cox  Karen Lowrie 《Risk analysis》2015,35(10):1765-1765
  相似文献   
49.
From the Editors     
Tony Cox  Karen Lowrie 《Risk analysis》2015,35(8):1389-1392
  相似文献   
50.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号