全文获取类型
收费全文 | 12398篇 |
免费 | 220篇 |
专业分类
管理学 | 1743篇 |
民族学 | 58篇 |
人才学 | 1篇 |
人口学 | 1245篇 |
丛书文集 | 64篇 |
教育普及 | 1篇 |
理论方法论 | 1058篇 |
现状及发展 | 1篇 |
综合类 | 137篇 |
社会学 | 5959篇 |
统计学 | 2351篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 86篇 |
2021年 | 68篇 |
2020年 | 188篇 |
2019年 | 252篇 |
2018年 | 287篇 |
2017年 | 404篇 |
2016年 | 320篇 |
2015年 | 201篇 |
2014年 | 268篇 |
2013年 | 2117篇 |
2012年 | 432篇 |
2011年 | 349篇 |
2010年 | 297篇 |
2009年 | 277篇 |
2008年 | 310篇 |
2007年 | 313篇 |
2006年 | 287篇 |
2005年 | 294篇 |
2004年 | 286篇 |
2003年 | 246篇 |
2002年 | 270篇 |
2001年 | 310篇 |
2000年 | 304篇 |
1999年 | 286篇 |
1998年 | 196篇 |
1997年 | 186篇 |
1996年 | 182篇 |
1995年 | 176篇 |
1994年 | 141篇 |
1993年 | 161篇 |
1992年 | 178篇 |
1991年 | 160篇 |
1990年 | 167篇 |
1989年 | 186篇 |
1988年 | 162篇 |
1987年 | 145篇 |
1986年 | 162篇 |
1985年 | 178篇 |
1984年 | 175篇 |
1983年 | 169篇 |
1982年 | 130篇 |
1981年 | 108篇 |
1980年 | 107篇 |
1979年 | 144篇 |
1978年 | 93篇 |
1977年 | 88篇 |
1976年 | 90篇 |
1975年 | 84篇 |
1974年 | 83篇 |
1973年 | 74篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Existence and Nash implementation of efficient sharing rules for a commonly owned technology 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Suppose that a group of individuals owns collectively a technology which produces a consumption good by means of a (possibly
heterogeneous) input. A sharing rule associates input contributions with a vector of consumptions that are technologically
feasible. We show that the set of allocations obtained by any continuous sharing rule contains Pareto efficient allocations.
We also present a mechanism that implements in Nash equilibrium the Pareto efficient allocations contained in an arbitrary
sharing rule.
Received: 29 June 1998/Accepted: 15 November 2000 相似文献
2.
Steven M. Harris 《Journal of marital and family therapy》2002,28(3):383-387
Bailey, C. E. (Ed.) (2000). Children in therapy: Using the family as a resource. Doherty, W. J. (2001). Take back your marriage: Sticking together in a world that pulls us apart. New York: Guilford. Larson, J. H. (2000). Should we stay together? A scientically proven method for evaluating your relationship and improving its chances for long‐term success. Weiner‐Davis, M. (2001). The divorce remedy: The proven 7‐step program for saving your marriage. 相似文献
3.
The authors examined the impact of sense of coherence on the career thought processes of a sample of college students. A. Antonovsky (1987) defined sense of coherence as the global orientation that the world is comprehensible, manageable, and meaningful. Study participants completed the Sense of Coherence Scale (A. Antonovsky, 1987) and the Career Thoughts Inventory (CTI; J. P. Sampson, G. W. Peterson, J. G. Lenz, R. C. Reardon, & D. E. Saunders, 1996). Multivariate tests indicated a medium relationship between sense of coherence and CTI Total and subscale scores; sense of coherence accounted for 14% of the variance. Univariate tests also indicated a medium relationship between sense of coherence and each subscale. 相似文献
4.
Colin C. Williams 《The Sociological review》2002,50(4):525-542
A recurring theme across the social sciences is that non‐capitalist production is disappearing albeit slowly and unevenly, and is being replaced by a commodified economy in which goods and services are produced by capitalist firms for a profit under conditions of market exchange. In this paper, however, I evaluate critically this commodification thesis. Even in the heartland of commoditisation ‐ the advanced economies. Large economic spaces are identified where alternative economic relations and motives prevail. Rather than view them as leftovers of pre‐capitalist formations, this paper argues that they are the result of both the contradictions inherent in the structural shifts associated with the pursuit of commodification as well as the existence of‘cultures of resistance’, As such, they are viewed as 'spaces of hope’which highlight the demonstrable construction and practice of alternative social relations and logic's of work outside profit‐motivated market‐oriented exchange. 相似文献
5.
6.
7.
Fire and restoration of the largest urban forest of the world in Rio de Janeiro City,Brazil 总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0
National Park of Tijuca in Rio de Janeiro (Brazil) is about 3,300 ha and considered the largest urban forest in the world. Its floristic composition is typical of Atlantic Rain Forest. The reserve is being altered because of fire occurrences and urban expansion. This study identified locations and causes of forest fires, and makes management recommendations to restore damaged areas. From 1991 to 2000, forest firefighters recorded an average of 75-fire occurrences/year. Identified causes included hot air balloons (24%), intentional (24%), rubbish burning (21%) and religious practices (17%). Primary fuels included invasive grasses and ferns. Although hot air balloons destroyed larger areas of forest in each occurrence, a greater number of fires started in the invasive vegetation along roads that bisect the forest. In response to recurrent forests, invasive vegetation has spread gradually into the forest increasing forest degradation. To decrease fire damage, sites with high fire frequencies and density of invasive vegetation were planted with less flammable species. Results indicate that fire frequency decreased and density of invasive vegetation declined. This approach appears to prevent fire incidence, reduce the need for fire fighting, and preserve existing biodiversity. 相似文献
8.
C. Eddie Palmer E. Joseph Savoie 《Sociological Practice: A Journal of Clinical and Applied Sociology》2002,4(1):89-97
Service-learning programs are not free from challenges brought about by lack of financial support, lack of widespread commitment from professors, community agencies, and recipients of service, and lack of knowledge and insight in students directly involved in such programs. While service-learning initiatives and programs serve positive functions for organizations and individuals, rhetorical accolades for service learning can distort or omit the realities of program implementation and sustained delivery. This paper specifically explores the following challenges connected to service-learning programs: (1) pedagogical difficulties; (2) student limitations; (3) time constraints; and (4) community cooperation. 相似文献
9.
Time, Self, and the Curiously Abstract Concept of Agency* 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
The term "agency" is quite slippery and is used differently depending on the epistemological roots and goals of scholars who employ it. Distressingly, the sociological literature on the concept rarely addresses relevant social psychological research. We take a social behaviorist approach to agency by suggesting that individual temporal orientations are underutilized in conceptualizing this core sociological concept. Different temporal foci—the actor's engaged response to situational circumstances—implicate different forms of agency. This article offers a theoretical model involving four analytical types of agency ("existential,""identity,""pragmatic," and "life course") that are often conflated across treatments of the topic. Each mode of agency overlaps with established social psychological literatures, most notably about the self, enabling scholars to anchor overly abstract treatments of agency within established research literatures. 相似文献
10.
Two habituation experiments were conducted to investigate how 4‐month‐old infants perceive partly occluded shapes. In the first experiment, we presented a simple, partly occluded shape to the infants until habituation was reached. Then we showed either a probable completion (one that would be predicted on the basis of both local and global cues) or an improbable completion. Longer looking times were found for the improbably completed shape (compared to probable and control conditions), suggesting that the probable shape was perceived during partial occlusion. In the second experiment, infants were habituated to more ambiguous partly occluded shapes, where local and global cues would result in different completions. For adults, the percept of these shapes is usually dominated by global influences. However, after habituation the infants looked longer at the globally completed shapes. These results suggest that by the age of 4 months, infants are able to infer the perceptual completion of partly occluded shapes, but for more ambiguous shapes, this completion seems to be dominated by local influences. 相似文献