全文获取类型
收费全文 | 20727篇 |
免费 | 380篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
管理学 | 2884篇 |
民族学 | 80篇 |
人才学 | 1篇 |
人口学 | 2038篇 |
丛书文集 | 79篇 |
教育普及 | 2篇 |
理论方法论 | 1919篇 |
现状及发展 | 1篇 |
综合类 | 267篇 |
社会学 | 10201篇 |
统计学 | 3636篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 118篇 |
2020年 | 309篇 |
2019年 | 411篇 |
2018年 | 482篇 |
2017年 | 657篇 |
2016年 | 523篇 |
2015年 | 375篇 |
2014年 | 505篇 |
2013年 | 3245篇 |
2012年 | 678篇 |
2011年 | 679篇 |
2010年 | 481篇 |
2009年 | 402篇 |
2008年 | 497篇 |
2007年 | 513篇 |
2006年 | 515篇 |
2005年 | 486篇 |
2004年 | 455篇 |
2003年 | 421篇 |
2002年 | 474篇 |
2001年 | 556篇 |
2000年 | 572篇 |
1999年 | 485篇 |
1998年 | 367篇 |
1997年 | 329篇 |
1996年 | 338篇 |
1995年 | 315篇 |
1994年 | 309篇 |
1993年 | 302篇 |
1992年 | 359篇 |
1991年 | 329篇 |
1990年 | 297篇 |
1989年 | 300篇 |
1988年 | 296篇 |
1987年 | 260篇 |
1986年 | 252篇 |
1985年 | 289篇 |
1984年 | 280篇 |
1983年 | 259篇 |
1982年 | 221篇 |
1981年 | 183篇 |
1980年 | 171篇 |
1979年 | 199篇 |
1978年 | 180篇 |
1977年 | 162篇 |
1976年 | 136篇 |
1975年 | 160篇 |
1974年 | 120篇 |
1973年 | 117篇 |
1971年 | 94篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
102.
Few researchers have examined organizational variation in the adoption of workplace drug testing, but innovation theory suggests that adoption is more likely when it is compatible with an organization's values, previously introduced ideas, and needs. Using data from the 1997 National Employee Survey, this research models the effects of organizational compatibility, industry, and establishment size on the adoption of drug testing. The data reveal that compatibility, as measured by an organization's rules orientation, presence of an employee assistance program, and mechanization, is associated with the adoption of drug testing. As predicted, the adoption of drug testing varies across industries and by establishment size. 相似文献
103.
104.
105.
Susan A. Ostrander 《Nonprofit management & leadership》2007,18(2):237-253
This case study looks at changes in private foundation funding for higher education civic engagement from 1995 to 2005. The focus is three large, established foundations that were U.S. national leaders in supporting this activity during these years: Carnegie, Pew, and Kellogg. Data sources include (1) in‐depth conversations with four strategically placed foundation insiders and six nationally recognized campus leaders and (2) scans of Web sites of the three foundations that document the change in funding priorities over time, plus Web sites of the main campus initiatives supported and national campus civic engagement organizations such as Campus Compact. Research findings con‐ firm and specify other studies that suggest that foundations shift their funding priorities according to their perceived role as innovators, pressures to be accountable and show clear results, and their independence as private organizations. The article concludes by raising questions about private foundations generally in relation to issues of innovativeness, accountability, and independence. 相似文献
106.
FINNEY DJ 《Journal of the American Statistical Association》1948,43(244):566-571
107.
Over the first years of life, infants gradually develop the ability to retrieve their memories across cue and contextual changes. Whereas maturational factors drive some of these developments in memory ability, experiences occurring within the learning event may also impact infants' ability to retrieve memories in new situations. In 2 experiments we examined whether it was possible to facilitate 12‐month‐old infants' generalization of learning in the deferred imitation paradigm by varying experiences before or during the demonstration session, or during the retention interval. In Experiment 1, altering the length, timing, or variability of training had no impact on generalization; infants showed a low, but consistent level of memory retrieval. In Experiment 2, infants who experienced a unique context for encoding and retrieval exhibited generalization; infants who experienced the context prior to the demonstration session, or during the retention interval, did not. Specificity is a robust feature of infant memory and is not substantially altered by encoding experiences in an observational learning paradigm. Previous history with a learning environment can, however, impact the flexibility of memory retrieval. 相似文献
108.
Paul J. E. Miller John P. Caughlin Ted L. Huston 《Journal of marriage and the family》2003,65(4):978-995
This study examined the processes that underlie the association between trait expressiveness and marital satisfaction. A total of 168 newlywed couples participated in a four‐wave, 13‐year longitudinal study of marriage. Cross‐sectional and longitudinal path analyses suggested that expressiveness promotes satisfaction by leading spouses to engage in affectionate behavior and by leading them to idealize their partner. Expressive people formed idealized images of their partner because they brought out the best in their partner's behavior and because they interpreted their partner's behavior in a favorable light. The study extends previous research by showing that the benefits of trait expressiveness extend into the second decade of marriage and by providing a plausible explanation of the connections between trait expressiveness and marital satisfaction. 相似文献
109.
The moderating role of maternal characteristics and maternal sensitivity on the concordance between maternal reports and laboratory observations of negative emotionality was examined. Participants were 90 primiparous mothers and their infants. Mothers completed questionnaires about remembered care from their own parents and their depressive symptoms prenatally, infant temperament (distress to novelty and distress to limitations), and depressive symptoms postnatally. Mothers and infants participated in a laboratory assessment of infant temperament (distress to novelty and limitations) and maternal sensitivity at 6 months postpartum. Several factors that moderate the degree of concordance between maternal reports and behavioral observations were identified, as predicted. Novelty concordance was higher when mothers reported having their emotional needs met in childhood and low prenatal depressive symptoms. Limitations concordance was higher when mothers were less sensitive during the observational tasks. Methodological and theoretical implications are discussed. 相似文献
110.
Nonlinguistic communication is typically proposed to convey representational messages, implying that particular signals are associated with specific signaler emotions, intentions, or external referents. However, common signals produced by both nonhuman primates and humans may not exhibit such specificity, with human laughter for example showing significant diversity in both acoustic form and production context. We therefore outline an alternative to the representational approach, arguing that laughter and other nonlinguistic vocalizations are used to influence the affective states of listeners, thereby also affecting their behavior. In the case of laughter, we propose a primary function of accentuating or inducing positive affect in the perceiver in order to promote a more favorable stance toward the laugher. Two simple strategies are identified, namely producing laughter with acoustic features that have an immediate impact on listener arousal, and pairing these sounds with positive affect in the listener to create learned affective responses. Both depend on factors like the listener's current emotional state and past interactions with the vocalizer, with laughers predicted to adjust their sounds accordingly. This approach is used to explain findings from two experimental studies that examined the use of laughter in same-sex and different-sex dyads composed of either friends or strangers, and may be applicable to other forms of nonlinguistic communication. 相似文献