全文获取类型
收费全文 | 123篇 |
免费 | 0篇 |
专业分类
管理学 | 9篇 |
民族学 | 1篇 |
人口学 | 3篇 |
丛书文集 | 39篇 |
理论方法论 | 14篇 |
综合类 | 48篇 |
社会学 | 6篇 |
统计学 | 3篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 1篇 |
2020年 | 1篇 |
2017年 | 1篇 |
2016年 | 1篇 |
2015年 | 2篇 |
2014年 | 3篇 |
2013年 | 7篇 |
2012年 | 10篇 |
2011年 | 10篇 |
2010年 | 5篇 |
2009年 | 12篇 |
2008年 | 8篇 |
2007年 | 10篇 |
2006年 | 10篇 |
2005年 | 6篇 |
2004年 | 7篇 |
2003年 | 9篇 |
2002年 | 8篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1999年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有123条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
121.
《Marriage & Family Review》2013,49(3-4):187-222
No abstract available for this article. 相似文献
122.
Belief system foundations of backward induction 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Two justifications of backward induction (BI) in generic perfect information games are formulated using Bonanno's (1992; Theory and Decision 33, 153) belief systems. The first justification concerns the BI strategy profile and is based on selecting a set of rational belief systems from which players have to choose their belief functions. The second justification concerns the BI path of play and is based on a sequential deletion of nodes that are inconsistent with the choice of rational belief functions. 相似文献
123.
Risk as Analysis and Risk as Feelings: Some Thoughts about Affect, Reason, Risk, and Rationality 总被引:16,自引:2,他引:16
Modern theories in cognitive psychology and neuroscience indicate that there are two fundamental ways in which human beings comprehend risk. The “analytic system” uses algorithms and normative rules, such as probability calculus, formal logic, and risk assessment. It is relatively slow, effortful, and requires conscious control. The “experiential system” is intuitive, fast, mostly automatic, and not very accessible to conscious awareness. The experiential system enabled human beings to survive during their long period of evolution and remains today the most natural and most common way to respond to risk. It relies on images and associations, linked by experience to emotion and affect (a feeling that something is good or bad). This system represents risk as a feeling that tells us whether it is safe to walk down this dark street or drink this strange‐smelling water. Proponents of formal risk analysis tend to view affective responses to risk as irrational. Current wisdom disputes this view. The rational and the experiential systems operate in parallel and each seems to depend on the other for guidance. Studies have demonstrated that analytic reasoning cannot be effective unless it is guided by emotion and affect. Rational decision making requires proper integration of both modes of thought. Both systems have their advantages, biases, and limitations. Now that we are beginning to understand the complex interplay between emotion and reason that is essential to rational behavior, the challenge before us is to think creatively about what this means for managing risk. On the one hand, how do we apply reason to temper the strong emotions engendered by some risk events? On the other hand, how do we infuse needed “doses of feeling” into circumstances where lack of experience may otherwise leave us too “coldly rational”? This article addresses these important questions. 相似文献