首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   879篇
  免费   27篇
  国内免费   4篇
管理学   83篇
民族学   1篇
人口学   24篇
丛书文集   30篇
理论方法论   66篇
综合类   96篇
社会学   129篇
统计学   481篇
  2024年   1篇
  2023年   14篇
  2022年   4篇
  2021年   12篇
  2020年   38篇
  2019年   43篇
  2018年   47篇
  2017年   48篇
  2016年   36篇
  2015年   21篇
  2014年   25篇
  2013年   213篇
  2012年   78篇
  2011年   35篇
  2010年   29篇
  2009年   23篇
  2008年   30篇
  2007年   22篇
  2006年   23篇
  2005年   28篇
  2004年   23篇
  2003年   17篇
  2002年   16篇
  2001年   18篇
  2000年   9篇
  1999年   10篇
  1998年   11篇
  1997年   6篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   2篇
  1994年   2篇
  1991年   1篇
  1990年   3篇
  1989年   1篇
  1988年   4篇
  1987年   2篇
  1985年   1篇
  1983年   2篇
  1982年   2篇
  1981年   2篇
  1980年   2篇
  1979年   2篇
  1978年   1篇
排序方式: 共有910条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
This study investigates how individuals assess imprecise information. We focus on two essential dimensions of decision under uncertainty, outcomes and probabilities, and their respective precision. We believe the precision of information is highly relevant in the investment setting, as reflected in the well-known “home (familiarity) bias”, and the outcome and probability dimensions, separately or jointly, may affect investors’ knowledge of uncertainty and perceived risk of the investment options, and subsequently affect investors’ choices. To test this conjecture, we conducted three experiments. Our results show that 1) participants demonstrate a pattern of preference for precision and aversion of extreme vagueness and associate vagueness with higher perceived risk and lower investment (experiments one and two); 2) participants prefer vague outcome information to vague probability information (experiment two); 3) familiarity indeed positively affects the precision of estimated values, but this association is stronger for the outcome dimension than for probabilities (experiment three). Our results confirm that precision in information, especially in the outcome dimension has an impact on investors’ resource allocation choices.  相似文献   
2.
Generally, the semiclosed-form option pricing formula for complex financial models depends on unobservable factors such as stochastic volatility and jump intensity. A popular practice is to use an estimate of these latent factors to compute the option price. However, in many situations this plug-and-play approximation does not yield the appropriate price. This article examines this bias and quantifies its impacts. We decompose the bias into terms that are related to the bias on the unobservable factors and to the precision of their point estimators. The approximated price is found to be highly biased when only the history of the stock price is used to recover the latent states. This bias is corrected when option prices are added to the sample used to recover the states' best estimate. We also show numerically that such a bias is propagated on calibrated parameters, leading to erroneous values. The Canadian Journal of Statistics 48: 8–35; 2020 © 2019 Statistical Society of Canada  相似文献   
3.
Abstract

The mean estimators with ratio depend on multiple auxiliary variables and unknown parameters in a finite population setting. We propose a new generalized approach with matrices for modeling the mutivariate mean estimators with two auxiliary variables. Our approach brings naturally a graphical analysis for comparing mean estimators.  相似文献   
4.
虚拟空间交织是人们常用的思维与认知方式之一。本文通过对交织及其原则的介绍,描述了虚拟空间交织的运作,指出虚拟空间交织构建中主观取向的原则应为最佳关联。  相似文献   
5.
Polynomial spline regression models of low degree have proved useful in modeling responses from designed experiments in science and engineering when simple polynomial models are inadequate. Where there is uncertainty in the number and location of the knots, or breakpoints, of the spline, then designs that minimize the systematic errors resulting from model misspecification may be appropriate. This paper gives a method for constructing such all‐bias designs for a single variable spline when the distinct knots in the assumed and true models come from some specified set. A class of designs is defined in terms of the inter‐knot intervals and sufficient conditions are obtained for a design within this class to be all‐bias under linear, quadratic and cubic spline models. An example of the construction of all‐bias designs is given.  相似文献   
6.
Local linear curve estimators are typically constructed using a compactly supported kernel, which minimizes edge effects and (in the case of the Epanechnikov kernel) optimizes asymptotic performance in a mean square sense. The use of compactly supported kernels can produce numerical problems, however. A common remedy is ridging, which may be viewed as shrinkage of the local linear estimator towards the origin. In this paper we propose a general form of shrinkage, and suggest that, in practice, shrinkage be towards a proper curve estimator. For the latter we propose a local linear estimator based on an infinitely supported kernel. This approach is resistant against selection of too large a shrinkage parameter, which can impair performance when shrinkage is towards the origin. It also removes problems of numerical instability resulting from using a compactly supported kernel, and enjoys very good mean squared error properties.  相似文献   
7.
基于异质投资者框架对大众媒体新闻、新媒体新闻与中国股市收益波动的影响进行探究,研究结果发现:(1)平均看来,新媒体新闻、大众媒体新闻与收益波动都变现为显著的正相关关系,且新媒体新闻与收益波动的平均相关性大于大众媒体新闻与收益波动的平均相关性。(2)具有高前期收益,高账面市值比,高机构投资者持股比例与高换手率这样的能引起媒体关注的特征的公司,其收益波动与新媒体新闻的相关性大于与大众媒体新闻相关系的概率更大。(3)对于小市值与年轻的公司,信息不确定程度高,新媒体新闻与其收益波动的相关性更可能大于大众媒体新闻与其收益波动的相关性。这一结果可以归因于新媒体新闻丰富了投资者的信息环境,更能影响投资者的异质信念水平。  相似文献   
8.
编辑偏见的存在有利有弊。编辑偏见有“有意识”与“无意识”之别。在编辑实践中,诱导编辑无意识偏见的发生因素主要有:认知懒惰或繁忙,非理性决策思维以及直觉信心等。编辑无意识偏见存在影响了编辑对稿件内容的客观评价,但因无意识编辑偏见具有较强的隐蔽性,消除比较困难,编辑必须通过无意识偏见发生前的“有意识”认知、严格落实“三审制”和利用大数据反馈等措施规避无意识偏见发生,以确保稿件价值选择的客观与公正。  相似文献   
9.
This study focuses on levels of concern for hurricanes among individuals living along the Gulf Coast during the quiescent two‐year period following the exceptionally destructive 2005 hurricane season. A small study of risk perception and optimistic bias was conducted immediately following Hurricanes Katrina and Rita. Two years later, a follow‐up was done in which respondents were recontacted. This provided an opportunity to examine changes, and potential causal ordering, in risk perception and optimistic bias. The analysis uses 201 panel respondents who were matched across the two mail surveys. Measures included hurricane risk perception, optimistic bias for hurricane evacuation, past hurricane experience, and a small set of demographic variables (age, sex, income, and education). Paired t‐tests were used to compare scores across time. Hurricane risk perception declined and optimistic bias increased. Cross‐lagged correlations were used to test the potential causal ordering between risk perception and optimistic bias, with a weak effect suggesting the former affects the latter. Additional cross‐lagged analysis using structural equation modeling was used to look more closely at the components of optimistic bias (risk to self vs. risk to others). A significant and stronger potentially causal effect from risk perception to optimistic bias was found. Analysis of the experience and demographic variables’ effects on risk perception and optimistic bias, and their change, provided mixed results. The lessening of risk perception and increase in optimistic bias over the period of quiescence suggest that risk communicators and emergency managers should direct attention toward reversing these trends to increase disaster preparedness.  相似文献   
10.
Altruism among humans is common. It is especially prominent among in-group members. However, we are struck by results from laboratory-in-the-field experiments with out-group members of different ethnic or religious backgrounds. In all instances, the groups were rivals in civil wars. While we find almost no altruistic behavior among groups that were engaged in fighting, out-group altruism emerges with the passage of time. To address the underlying mechanism that might explain this puzzling result, we use a laboratory experiment that sorts between a norms revision explanation and a contact hypothesis explanation. Our findings show that contact with out-group members, in a mutually beneficial task, results in a reversion to prior levels of out-group altruism following conflict. Our findings reinforce the idea that deeply held attitudes toward a stigmatized out-group change following productive interactions with those out-group members. It is clear that hostility between groups need not persist over time.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号